1701562967
1701562968
6.“outside memory store”: J. K. O’Regan (1992). “Solving the ‘Real’ Mysteries of Visual Perception: The World as an Outside Memory.”Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie46(3): 461–488.
1701562969
1701562970
7.What experimenters were wearing: E. S. Parker, L. Cahill, and J. L. McGaugh (2006). “A Case of Unusual Autobiographical Remembering.”Neurocase12 (1): 35–49.
1701562971
1701562972
8.calculating trajectories: A strategy like this for catching balls (to solve what vision scientists call the outfielder problem) was proposed by B. V. H. Saxberg (1987). “Projected Free Fall Trajectories. I. Theory and Simulation.”Biological Cybernetics: 56(2–3): 159–175.
1701562973
1701562974
9.angle is always increasing at a constant rate: A strategy first suggested by S. Chapman (1968). “Catching a Baseball.”American Journal of Physics36(10): 868–870.
1701562975
1701562976
10.players catching real balls: P. McLeod and Z. Dienes (1993). “Running to Catch the Ball.”Nature362(6415): 23; P. McLeod and Z. Dienes (1996). “Do Fielders Know Where to Go to Catch the Ball or Only How to Get There?”Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance22(3): 531–543.
1701562977
1701562978
11.chasing virtual balls: P. W. Fink, P. S. Foo, and W. H. Warren (2009). “Catching Fly Balls in Virtual Reality: A Critical Test of the Outfielder Problem.”Journal of Vision9(13): 14.
1701562979
1701562980
12.the side with the faster flow: A. P. Duchon and W. H. Warren Jr. (2002). “A Visual Equalization Strategy for Locomotor Control: Of Honeybees, Robots, and Humans.”Psychological Science13(3): 272–278.
1701562981
1701562982
13.Bees … slower optic flow: M. V. Srinivasan, M. Lehrer, W. H. Kirchner, and S. W. Zhang (1991). “Range Perception Through Apparent Image Speed in Freely Flying Honeybees.”Visual Neuroscience6(5): 519–535.
1701562983
1701562984
14.it’s in the brain: We were inspired to ask this question and present the material this way by a talk entitled “Cognitive Ethnography” by Edwin Hutchins at the Cognitive Science Society conference that took place in Boston in 2003. A more recent articulation of Hutchins’s views about the relation between cognition, culture, and the environment can be found in E. Hutchins (2014). “The Cultural Ecosystem of Human Cognition.”Philosophical Psychology27(1): 34–49.
1701562985
1701562986
15.Experiment to judge orientation: M. Tucker and R. Ellis (1998). “On the Relations Between Seen Objects and Components of Potential Actions.”Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance24(3): 830–846.
1701562987
1701562988
16.memorization techniques: C. L. Scott, R. J. Harris, and A. R. Rothe (2001). “Embodied Cognition Through Improvisation Improves Memory for a Dramatic Monologue.”Discourse Processes31(3): 293–305.
1701562989
1701562990
17.embodiment: These ideas took on prominence due to the work of a number of people, including Lawrence Barsalou and Arthur Glenberg.
1701562991
1701562992
18.Oksapmin people: G. B. Saxe (1981). “Body Parts as Numerals: A Developmental Analysis of Numeration Among the Oksapmin in Papua New Guinea.”Child Development52(1): 306–316.
1701562993
1701562994
19.unified with the objects that we’re thinking about and with: A fuller presentation of these ideas can be found in M. Wilson (2002). “Six Views of Embodied Cognition.”Psychonomic Bulletin & Review9(4): 625–636.
1701562995
1701562996
20.somatic markers: This idea is spelled out in A. R. Damasio (1994).Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason and the Human Brain. New York: G. P. Putnam’s.
1701562997
1701562998
21.moral reactions: This is an idea made popular by J. Haidt (2001). “The Emotional Dog and Its Rational Tail: A Social Intuitionist Approach to Moral Judgment.”Psychological Review108(4): 814–834.
1701562999
1701563000
第六章 他人的智慧
1701563001
1701563002
1.Speth describes communal bison hunts:J. D. Speth (1997). “Communal Bison Hunting in Western North America: Background for the Study of Paleolithic Bison Hunting in Europe.”L’Alimentation des Hommes du Paléolitique83: 23–57, ERAUL, Liége.
1701563003
1701563004
2.brain mass of modern humans:S. Shultz, E. Nelson, and R. I. Dunbar (2012). “Hominin Cognitive Evolution: Identifying Patterns and Processes in the Fossil and Archeological Record.”Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences367(1599): 2130–2140.
1701563005
1701563006
3.physically weaker to compensate
:www.nytimes.com/2014/05/28/ science/ stronger-brains-weaker-bodies.html?_r=0.
1701563007
1701563008
4.snowball effect:A. Whiten and D. Erdal (2012). “The Human Socio-Cognitive Niche and Its Evolutionary Origins.”Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences367(1599): 2119–2129.
1701563009
1701563010
5.Hunting … instrumental to human evolution: R. Ardrey (1976).The Hunting Hypothesis: A Personal Conclusion Concerning the Evolutionary Nature of Man. New York: Atheneum.
1701563011
1701563012
6.Robin Dunbar, social brain hypothesis: R. I. Dunbar (1992). “Neocortex Size as a Constraint on Group Size in Primates.”Journal of Human Evolution22(6): 469–493.
1701563013
1701563014
7.reasoning about intentionality
:For a penetrating analysis of what this kind of reasoning requires, see B. F. Malle and J. Knobe (1997). “The Folk Concept of Intentionality.”Journal of Experimental Social Psychology33(2): 101–121.
1701563015
1701563016
8.Tomasello, shared intentionality: This and the other work on shared intentionality reviewed here are discussed in M. Tomasello and M. Carpenter (2007). “Shared Intentionality.”Developmental Science10(1): 121–125.
[
上一页 ]
[ :1.701562967e+09 ]
[
下一页 ]