打字猴:1.701503046e+09
1701503046
1701503047 (285) William E. Connolly, “Critical Response I: The Complexity of Intention”, Critical Inquiry , 37/4 (2011), 791—798; Ruth Leys, “Critical Response II: Affect and Intention: A Reply to William E. Connolly”, Critical Inquiry , 37/4 (2011), 799—805.
1701503048
1701503049 (286) William E. Connolly, “Materialities of Experience”, in Diana H. Coole and Samantha Frost (eds.), New Materialisms: Ontology, Agency, and Politics (Durham, NC: Duke University Press, 2010), 198 n. 6.
1701503050
1701503051 (287) 人们也不能像约翰· 普罗特维那样,从严格的哲学角度为躯体标记假说辩护;“肖恩· 加拉格尔(Shaun Gallagher)会说,达马西奥假设的细节‘要么以一种现象学上站不住脚的方式将身体夸大到思想的层面,要么将其简化为神经元过程’。但是如果我们考虑到科隆贝蒂(Colombetti)和汤普森(Thompson)的提醒,模拟身体回路需要持续不断的体细胞更新,因此并不仅仅是神经中枢的表现,我们可以保留‘躯体标记’假说”,见Protevi, Political Affect , 198 n. 35。
1701503052
1701503053 (288) Slavoj Žižek, “Have Michael Hardt and Antonio Negri Rewritten the Communist Manifesto for the Twenty-First Century?”, Rethinking Marxism , 13/3—4 (2001), 190—198.
1701503054
1701503055 (289) Michael Hardt and Antonio Negri, Multitude: War And Democracy in the Age of Empire (New York: Penguin Books, 2005), 109,原文强调。也见Michael Hardt, “Affective Labor”,Boundary 2 , 26/2 (1999), 89—100; Michael Hardt, “Foreword: What Affects Are Good For”,in Patricia Ticineto Clough and Jean Halley (eds.), The Affective Turn: Theorizing the Social (Durham, NC: Duke University Press, 2007), ix—xiii。也见Michael Hardt and Antonio Negri, Empire (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2000)。
1701503056
1701503057 (290) Hardt and Negri, Multitude , 108.
1701503058
1701503059 (291) 斯宾诺莎、德勒兹、马苏米,见Hardt, “Foreword”, xiii n. 2;达马西奥,见Hardt and Negri, Multitude , 374 n. 9。
1701503060
1701503061 (292) Hardt, “Foreword”, x.
1701503062
1701503063 (293) Hardt, “Foreword”, xii.
1701503064
1701503065 (294) Hardt, “Affective Labor”, 100.
1701503066
1701503067 (295) Adrian Johnston, “The Misfeeling of What Happens: Slavoj Žižek, Antonio Damasio and a Materialist Account of Affects”, Subjectivity , 3/1 (2010), 76—100; Slavoj Žižek, “Some Concluding Notes on Violence, Ideology and Communist Culture”, Subjectivity , 3/1 (2010), 101—116.
1701503068
1701503069 (296) Walter Benn Michaels, The Shape of the Signifier: 1967 to the End of History (Princeton:Princeton University Press, 2004), 9—10.
1701503070
1701503071 (297) Michaels, The Shape of the Signifier , 180—181,原文强调。
1701503072
1701503073 (298) Jacob Weisberg, The Bush Tragedy (New York: Random House, 2008). “Compassionate conservatism” is outlined, for example, in Marvin Olasky, Compassionate Conservatism: What it is,What it Does, and How it Can Transform America (New York: Free Press, 2000); Arthur C. Brooks,Who Really Cares: The Surprising Truth About Compassionate Conservatism (New York: Basic Books, 2006).
1701503074
1701503075 (299) 这似乎是合适的,见Patricia T. Clough, “The Affective Turn: Political Economy, Biomedia and Bodies”, Theory, Culture & Society , 25/1 (2008), 1—22, esp. 15—17。
1701503076
1701503077 (300) 自然科学的认识论(特别是证伪问题)本身就是一个研究领域。在此我们只提一点,科学哲学家伊姆雷· 拉卡托斯(Imre Lakatos)对卡尔· 波普尔(Karl Popper)的证伪主义的修正如下(在托马斯· 库恩[Thomas Kuhn]那里为他辩护):一般来说,只有特定的陈述和预测被证伪,全盘否定整个研究方案非常罕见。见Imre Lakatos,The Methodology of Scientific Research Programmes (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1978), 8—101; Imre Lakatos, Proofs and Refutations: the Logic of Mathematical Discovery (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1976)。关于这场争论,见Gunnar Andersson,Kritik und Wissenschaftsgeschichte: Kuhns, Lakatos’ und Feyerabends Kritik des Kritischen Rationalismus (Tübingen: Mohr, 1988)。一篇总结社会科学和神经科学在认识论上的异同的有用的文章,见John Cromby, “Integrating Social Science with Neuroscience: Potentials and Problems”,BioSocieties , 2/2 (2007), 149—169。
1701503078
1701503079 (301) 钟形曲线是人类群体中智商分布符合钟形的正态曲线,是赫恩斯坦(Herrnstein)和默里(Murray)的著作的书名,书中声称美国黑人的平均智力水平低于美国白人。Richard J. Herrnstein and Charles Murray, The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life (New York: Free Press, 1994)。
1701503080
1701503081 (302) Jan Plamper, “The History of Emotions: An Interview with William Reddy, Barbara H. Rosenwein, and Peter Sterns by Jan Plamper”, History and Theory , 49/2 (2010), 237—265,here 248.
1701503082
1701503083 (303) 虽然在神经科学领域流行的英语文本几乎总是很快被翻译成德语,但几乎没有类似的德语著作被翻译成英语。见Gerhard Roth, Fühlen, Denken, Handeln: Wie das Gehirnunser Verhaltensteuert (Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, 2001); Gerhard Roth, Aus Sicht des Gehirns (Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, 2003); Gerhard Roth, Persönlichkeit, Entscheidung und Verhalten:Warum es so schwierig ist, sich und andere zu ändern (Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta, 2007); Michael Pauen and Gerhard Roth, Freiheit, Schuld und Verantwortung: Grundzüge einer naturalistischen Theorie der Willensfreiheit (Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, 2008); Wolf Singer, Der Beobachter im Gehirn: Essays zur Hirnforschung (Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, 2002); Wolf Singer, Ein neues Menschenbild? Gespräche über Hirnforschung (Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, 2003); Wolf Singer,Vom Gehirn zum Bewußtsein (Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, 2006); Hans J. Markowitsch and Werner Siefer, Tatort Gehirn: Auf der Suchenach dem Ursprung des Verbrechens (Frankfurt am Main: Campus, 2007); Hans J. Markowitsch, Das Gedächtnis: Entwicklung, Funktionen, Störungen (Munich: Beck, 2009); Manfred Spitzer, Nervensachen: Geschichten vom Gehirn (Frankfurt am Main: Suhrkamp, 2005); Manfred Spitzer, Dopamin & Käsekuchen: Hirnforschung à la carte (Stuttgart: Schattauer, 2011); Manfred Spitzer, Nichtstun, Flirten, Küssen und andereLeistungen des Gehirns (Stuttgart: Schattauer, 2012); Manfred Spitzer, Digitale Demenz: Wie wir uns und unsere Kinder um den Verstand bringen (Munich: Droemer, 2012)。例外的是Hans J. Markowitsch and Harald Welzer, The Development of Autobiographical Memory , trans. David Emmans (Hove:Psychology Press, 2010) [Ger. orig., Das autobiographische Gedächtnis: Hirnorganische Grundlagen und biosoziale Entwicklung , 2005]; Birgitt Röttger-Rössler and Hans J. Markowitsch (eds.),Emotions as Bio-Cultural Processes (New York: Springer, 2009)。一本是在德语原版出版五年后出版的,另一本是一个以英语为母语的研究小组的论文合集。
1701503084
1701503085 (304) Thomas Metzinger cited in Schnabel, “Wie Agenten, Verleger und Autoren mit populärer Welterklärung Gewinn machen”, 43.
1701503086
1701503087 (305) Jean Delumeau, La Peur en Occident (ⅩⅥe—ⅩⅤⅢe siècles): Une cité assiégée (Paris: Fayard, 1978), 13.又见斯特恩斯夫妇,他们将20世纪80年代初期生命科学对情感的认识总结为“腺体或激素反应”;见Peter N. Stearns and Carol Z. Stearns,“ Emotionology:Clarifying the History of Emotions and Emotional Standards”, American Historical Review , 90/4 (1985), 813—836, here 834。
1701503088
1701503089 (306) 借自柏林马克斯· 普朗克科学史研究所的网站:; 也见Choudhury and Slaby, Critical Neuroscience ; William R. Uttal, Mind and Brain: A Critical Appraisal of Cognitive Neuroscience (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 2011),探讨情感的第4章。又见批评的博客:Michael Anderson, “After Phrenology: Will we Ever Understand the Brain”,Psychology Today ; Luiz Pessoa,“Emotion & Cognition and the Brain”, ; Michelle Greene, NeurRealism ); The Neurocritic: Deconstructing the Most Sensationalistic Recent Findings in Human Brain Imaging, Cognitive Neuroscience, and Psychopharmacology ; “Neuroskeptic”, Discover ; all accessed 16 March 2014。
1701503090
1701503091 (307) Ekman and Davidson (eds.), Nature of Emotion ; Richard J. Davidson, “Seven Sins in the Study of Emotion: Correctives from Affective Neuroscience”, Brain and Cognition , 52/1(2003), 129—132; JustinS. Feinstein, Ralph Adolphs, Antonio Damasio, and Daniel Tranel,“The Human Amygdala and the Induction and Experience of Fear”, Current Biology , 21/1(2011), 34—38; Pessoa and Adolphs, “Emotion Processing and the Amygdala” .
1701503092
1701503093 (308) “They Found God and Other Spiritual Phenomena in the Temporal Lobe”, Personality Cafe accessed 16 March 2014; 见Martin Haidinger,“Die Amygdala regelt im Gehirn das Sozialleben”, in Wissen aktuell (OE1 on ORF, 27 December 2010, 1.55 p.m.) [broadcast]。
1701503094
1701503095 (309) 这种趋势的典型代表是路易斯· 佩索阿的元分析,“Emergent Processes in Cognitive-Emotional Interactions”, Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience , 12/4 (2010), 433—448,及Dalgleish, Dunn,and Mobbs, “Affective Neuroscience”, 363。“对情感神经科学发展的历史分析表明,涉及情感和情绪处理的大脑区域比最初设想的要多得多。”
[ 上一页 ]  [ :1.701503046e+09 ]  [ 下一页 ]