打字猴:1.701558289e+09
1701558289
1701558290 43这就是白日梦模式:Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Pow-ers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 676–682.
1701558291
1701558292 44 默认模式:Raichle, M. E., MacLeod, A. M., Snyder, A. Z., Powers, W. J., Gusnard, D. A., & Shulman, G. L. (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(2), 676–682.
1701558293
1701558294 44 神游模式: 在科学文献中,我所说的神游模式都是指预设模式或负任务网络;中央执行模式指正任务网络。
1701558295
1701558296 44 这两种大脑状态形成了阴阳两极
1701558297
1701558298 Binder, J. R., Frost, J. A., Hammeke, T. A., Bellgowan, P. S., Rao, S. M., & Cox, R. W. (1999). Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state: A functional MRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(1), 80–93.
1701558299
1701558300 Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.
1701558301
1701558302 Fox, M. D., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Corbetta, M., Van Essen, D. C., & Raichle, M. E. (2005). The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(27), 9673–9678.
1701558303
1701558304 Mazoyer, B., Zago, L., Mellet, E., Bricogne, S., Etard, O., Houde, O., … Tzourio-Mazoyer, N. (2001). Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Research Bulletin, 54(3), 287–298.
1701558305
1701558306 Shulman, G. L., Fiez, J. A., Corbetta, M., Buckner, R. L., Miezin, F. M., Raichle, M. E., & Petersen, S. E. (1997). Common blood flow changes across visual tasks: II. Decreases in cerebral cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(5), 648–663.
1701558307
1701558308 44大脑神游网络被压抑得越多:Menon, V., & Uddin, L. Q. (2010). Saliency, switching, attention and control: A network model of insula function. Brain Structure and Function, 214( 5–6), 655–667.
1701558309
1701558310 44 注意力过滤器:为了更清楚、简约地说明问题,我在这里说明一下,神经科学文献考虑了三种不同的系统:过滤器本身,显著性检测器(也被称为定向系统或重新定位系统),以及警报或警觉模式。对神经科学家而言,它们之间的差别是很重要的,但对非专业人士而言,这一点也不重要。
1701558311
1701558312 44神游模式是一种网络:Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., & Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 100(1), 253–258.
1701558313
1701558314 47 这种转换机制是由大脑中的脑岛所控制:Sridharan, D., Levitin, D. J., & Menon, V. (2008). A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(34), 12569–12574.
1701558315
1701558316 脑岛也参与了注意力,因为它有助于调节身体与情感冲动。冲动通常是动态平衡缺失的信号,我们尤其需要注意这一点——例如,渴了或饿了就是明显的例子,或渴望蛋白或处于较冷的环境。但持续的关注,要求我们抑制这些冲动。我们有些人比其他人更擅长这个——对某些人来说,他们能够集中注意力,压制身体的不舒适;但对其他人而言,冲动战胜了一切,最终我们只能在冰箱上粘贴各种各样的提示。脑岛有助于平衡这些相互竞争的需求,它的一部分职责是当重要的冲动产生时,能够给意识发送信号。脑岛损伤的人更容易戒烟——因为冲动没有被向上传递到意识。
1701558317
1701558318 Naqvi, N. H., Rudrauf, D., Damasio, H., & Bechara, A. (2007). Damage to the insula disrupts addiction to cigarette smoking. Science, 315(5811), 531–534.
1701558319
1701558320 47外部两个物体之间的转换需要颞–顶枢纽的短暂参与:Corbetta, M., Patel, G., & Shulman, G. L. (2008). The reorienting system of the human brain: From environment to theory of mind. Neuron, 58(3), 306–324.
1701558321
1701558322 Shulman, G. L., & Corbetta, M. (2014). Two attentional networks: Identification and function within a larger cognitive architecture. In M. Posner (Ed.), The cognitive neuroscience of attention (2nd ed.) (pp. 113–128). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
1701558323
1701558324 其他观点,你还可以参见 Geng, J. J., & Vossel, S. (2013). Re-evaluating the role of TPJ in attentional control: Contextual updating? Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10), 2608–2620.
1701558325
1701558326 47 就像跷跷板: Meyer, M. L., Spunt, R. P., Berkman, E. T., Taylor, S. E., & Lieberman, M. D. (2012). Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(6), 1883–1888.
1701558327
1701558328 50 丹尼尔·丹尼特认为: Dennett, D. C. (1991). Consciousness explained. New York, NY: Little, Brown and Company.
1701558329
1701558330 51 通常一次最多只能容纳4~5件事情:长达几十年来,我们一直接受的是乔治·米勒于1956年发现的经典:注意力限制在7±2项,现在,这一观点已经让位当代神经科学的观点精确到仅仅只有4件事物。
1701558331
1701558332 Cowan, N. (2009). Capacity limits and consciousness. In T. Baynes, A. Cleeremans, & P. Wilken (Eds.), Oxford companion to consciousness (pp. 127–130). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
1701558333
1701558334 Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(1), 51–57.
1701558335
1701558336 51 人类注意力系统由4个部分组成: 认知神经学家识别了第五大部分,警告模式或警觉模式。这从概念上与注意力过滤器有所不同,但出于讨论的目的,我把它视为一种特殊的任务模式,人们在任务中保持搜索或警惕。
1701558337
1701558338 当你保持警觉的时候,你会处于这种状态——休息状态被一种新的状态取代,这涉及准备检测,响应预期的信号。当我们等待电话响起的时候,等待交通灯变绿的时候,等待另一只鞋子掉下来的时候,我们都会这样做。它的特征是警觉度、感官灵敏度、觉醒度水平都较高。
[ 上一页 ]  [ :1.701558289e+09 ]  [ 下一页 ]